Copas selection models for meta-analysis

effect size
distribution theory
selective reporting
Author

James E. Pustejovsky

Published

July 27, 2024

Recently, I’ve been studying and tinkering with meta-analytic models for selective reporting of findings from primary studies. I’ve mostly focused on step-function selection models (Hedges, 1992; Hedges & Vevea, 1996; Vevea & Hedges, 1995; Vevea & Woods, 2005), which assume that selective reporting of primary study results is driven by the \(p\)-values of the effect size estimates (and specifically, where those \(p\)-values fall relative to certain thresholds). This is not the only form of selection model, though. One prominent alternative model, described in a series of papers by John Copas and colleagues (Copas, 1999; Copas & Li, 1997; Copas & Shi, 2000, 2001), is based on distinctly different assumptions than the step-function model. The Copas selection model has received a fair amount of attention in the literature on meta-analysis of medical and epidemiological research, but relatively little attention from the social science side of things. In this post, I’ll give an overview of the Copas model and try to highlight some of the ways that it differs from step function selection models.

The Copas selection model

Similar to the step function selection model, the Copas model is composed of two components: an evidence-generating process that describes the distribution of effect size estimates prior to selection and a selective reporting process that describes how selective reporting happens as a function of the effect size estimates and associated quantities like standard errors and \(p\)-values. The evidence-generating process is just a conventional random effects model. Letting \(T_i^*\) denote an effect size estimate prior to selective reporting, \(\theta_i^*\) denote the corresponding effect size parameter, and \(\sigma_i^*\) denote its (known) standard error, we assume that effect size estimate \(i\) is drawn from a normal distribution with mean \(\theta_i^*\) and standard deviation \(\sigma_i^*\): \[ T_i^* | \theta_i^*, \sigma_i^* \sim N\left(\theta_i^*, \left(\sigma_i^*\right)^2\right). \tag{1}\] We further assume that the effect size parameters are drawn from a normal distribution with overall average effect size \(\mu\) and standard deviation \(\tau\): \[ \theta_i^* | \sigma_i^* \sim N\left(\mu, \tau^2\right). \tag{2}\] The full evidence-generating process can also be written as a random effects model with two sources of error: \[ T_i^* = \mu + \nu_i^* + \epsilon_i^*, \tag{3}\] where \(\nu_i^* \sim N(0, \tau^2)\) and \(\epsilon_i^* \sim N\left(0, \left(\sigma_i^*\right)^2\right)\).

The second component is where the Copas model diverges from the step-function model. The Copas model posits that selection is driven by a latent index \(\zeta_i\) that is normally distributed and correlated with \(\epsilon_i^*\), with mean that is a function of \(\sigma_i^*\): \[ \zeta_i = \alpha + \frac{\beta}{\sigma_i^*} + \delta_i, \tag{4}\] where \(\delta_i \sim N(0, 1)\) and \(\text{cor}(\delta_i, \epsilon_i^*) = \rho\). With this specification, \(T_i^*\) is reported (and thus available for inclusion in a meta-analysis) if \(\zeta_i > 0\) and is otherwise unreported. Following the notation of my previous post, I’ll use \(O_i^*\) to denote a binary indicator equal to 1 if \(T_i^*\) is reported and otherwise equal to zero. With this notation, \(\Pr\left(O_i^* = 1 | \sigma_i^*\right) = \Pr\left(\zeta_i > 0 | \sigma_i^*\right)\).

Copas & Shi (2001) calls \(\zeta_i\) the propensity for selection. Under the posited model, the selection propensity depends both on the precision of the study through \(\sigma_i^*\) (because the mean of \(\zeta_i\) depends on the inverse of \(\sigma_i^*\)) and on the magnitude of the reported effect size estimate (because \(\zeta_i\) is correlated with \(\epsilon_i^*\)). The parameter \(\alpha\) controls the overall probability of selection, irrespective of the findings; \(\beta\) controls how strongly selection depends on study precision; and \(\rho\) controls how strongly selection depends on the magnitude of the effect size estimate itself.

The Copas model posits a stable correlation \(\rho\) between the selection propensity index and the samplng error of the random effects model, \(\epsilon_i^*\). If there is heterogeneity in the effect size parameters and heterogeneity in the sampling variances, then the correlation between \(\zeta_i\) and \(T_i^*\) will be lower than \(\rho\) and will differ from study to study. With a little variance algebra, it can be seen that \[ \text{cor}\left(\zeta_i, T_i^* | \sigma_i^*\right) = \tilde\rho_i = \frac{\rho \sigma_i^*}{\sqrt{\tau^2 + \left(\sigma_i^*\right)^2}}. \tag{5}\] Using the correlation, we can write the selection propensity index in terms of a regression on \(T_i^*\): \[ \zeta_i | T_i^*, \sigma_i^* \sim N \left(\alpha + \frac{\beta}{\sigma_i^*} + \tilde\rho_i \left(\frac{T_i^* - \mu}{\sqrt{\tau^2 + \left(\sigma_i^*\right)^2}}\right), \ 1 - \rho_i^2\right). \] Thus, given the effect size estimate \(T_i^*\) and its standard error \(\sigma_i^*\), the probability that finding \(i\) is reported is \[ \Pr\left(O_i^* = 1 | T_i^*, \sigma_i^*\right) = \Phi\left(\frac{1}{\sqrt{1 - \tilde\rho_i^2}}\left[\alpha + \frac{\beta}{\sigma_i^*} + \tilde\rho_i \left(\frac{T_i^* - \mu}{\sqrt{\tau^2 + \left(\sigma_i^*\right)^2}}\right)\right]\right) \tag{6}\] (Copas & Shi, 2001, Equation 4; Hedges & Vevea, 2005, Equation 9.6).

Distribution of observed effect sizes

The observed effect size estimates \(T_i\) follow a distribution equivalent to that of \(\left(T_i^* | \sigma_i^*, O_i^* = 1\right)\). It’s possible to find an analytic expression for the density of the observed effect sizes. Using Bayes Theorem, the density of the observed \(T_i\)’s is \[ \Pr\left(T_i = t| \sigma_i^*\right) = \Pr\left(T_i^* = t | \sigma_i^*, O_i^* = 1\right) = \frac{\Pr\left(O_i^* = 1 | T_i^* = t, \sigma_i^*\right) \times \Pr\left(T_i^* = t | \sigma_i^*\right)}{\Pr\left(O_i^* = 1 | \sigma_i^*\right)}, \tag{7}\] where the first term in the numerator is given in (6), the second term in the numerator can be derived from (3), and the denominator can be derived from (4). Letting \(\eta_i = \sqrt{\tau^2 + \left(\sigma_i^*\right)^2}\) and substituting everything into the above gives \[ \Pr\left(T_i = t| \sigma_i^*\right) = \frac{1}{\eta_i} \phi\left(\frac{t - \mu}{\eta_i}\right) \times \frac{\Phi\left(\frac{1}{\sqrt{1 - \tilde\rho_i^2}}\left[\alpha + \frac{\beta}{\sigma_i^*} + \tilde\rho_i \left(\frac{t - \mu}{\eta_i}\right)\right]\right)} {\Phi\left(\alpha + \frac{\beta}{\sigma_i^*}\right)}. \tag{8}\] This density is identical to that of the generalized skew-normal distribution (Arellano-Valle & Azzalini, 2006).

Here is an interactive graph showing the distribution of the effects prior to selection (in grey) and the distribution of observed effect sizes (in blue) based on the Copas selection model.1 Initially, the selection parameters are set to \(\alpha = -3\), \(\beta = 0.5\) and \(\rho = 0.5\), but you can change these however you like. Try wiggling \(\rho\) up and down to see how the bias and shape of the density changes.

Moments of \(T_i | \sigma_i\)

Arnold et al. (1993) derived the moment-generating function and first several moments of a further generalization of the skew-normal distribution, of which the distribution of \(T_i\) is a special case. Letting \(Z_i = \frac{T_i - \mu}{\sqrt{\tau^2 + \sigma_i^2}}\), \(u_i = \alpha + \frac{\beta}{\sigma_i}\), and \(\lambda(u_i) = \phi(u_i) / \Phi(u_i)\), they give the first two moments of \(Z_i\) as \[ \begin{aligned} \mathbb{E}\left(Z_i | \sigma_i \right) &= \rho_i \lambda\left(u_i\right) \\ \mathbb{E}\left(Z_i^2 | \sigma_i \right) &= 1 - \rho_i^2 u_i \lambda(u_i) \end{aligned} \] by which it follows that \[ \begin{aligned} \mathbb{E}\left(T_i | \sigma_i \right) &= \mu + \sqrt{\tau^2 + \sigma_i^2} \times \rho_i \lambda\left(u_i\right) \\ &= \mu + \rho \sigma_i \lambda\left(u_i\right) \end{aligned} \tag{9}\] and \[ \begin{aligned} \mathbb{V}\left(T_i | \sigma_i \right) &= \left(\tau^2 + \sigma_i^2\right) \left(1 - \rho_i^2\lambda(u_i) \times [u_i + \lambda(u_i)]\right) \\ &= \tau^2 + \sigma_i^2\left(1 - \rho^2 \lambda(u_i) \times [u_i + \lambda(u_i)]\right). \end{aligned} \tag{10}\] The expression for \(\mathbb{E}\left(T_i | \sigma_i \right)\) is also given in Copas & Shi (2000) and Copas & Shi (2001).2

Several things are noteworthy about these expressions. For one, the expectation of \(T_i\) is additive in \(\mu\), so the bias \(\mathbb{E}(T_i | \sigma_i) - \mu\) does not depend on the location of the distribution prior to selection. Nor does it depend on the degree of heterogeneity \(\tau\). Likewise, the variance of \(T_i\) is additive in \(\tau^2\). Only the sampling variance is affected by selection. All of these properties are in contrast to the step-function selection model, where both the bias and the variance are complex functions that do depend on \(\mu\) and \(\tau\).

Under the Copas model, the bias of \(T_i\) is directly proportional to \(\rho\) and is a more complicated function of \(\alpha\) and \(\beta\) (through \(u_i\)). The bias is an increasing function of \(\sigma\), the shape of which depends on \(\alpha\) and \(\beta\). Here’s an illustration of \(\sigma_i \times \lambda(u_i)\) as a function of \(\sigma_i\) for a few different values of the selection parameters:

When \(\beta = 0\), the bias is directly proportional to \(\sigma_i\), just like in an Egger regression or the PET meta-regression adjustment method (Stanley, 2008). As \(\beta\) increases, the bias is initially very close to zero and then grows almost linearly after a certain point—much as in the kinked meta-regression adjustment proposed by Bom & Rachinger (2019). Very curious.

Funnel density

Here is an interactive funnel plot showing the distribution of effect size estimates under the Copas selection model. I initially set \(\mu = 0.15\) and \(\tau = 0.10\) and selection parameters of \(\alpha = -3\), \(\beta = 0.5\) and \(\rho = 0.5\), but you can change these however you like.

The funnel plot shows the un-normalized distribution of the observed effect sizes conditional on \(\sigma_i\). One of the very striking things about the funnel is just how steeply the marginal selection probabilities (i.e., \(\Pr(O_i^* = 1| \sigma_i^*)\)) drop off as \(\sigma_i\) increases (going from the top to the bottom of the plot). With the initial parameter settings, findings from small studies with large \(\sigma_i\)s are generally quite unlikely to be published, regardless of their significance or effect size. Increasing \(\alpha\) will increase the marginal selection probability for all studies, though in a non-linear way depending on their \(\sigma_i\)’s. Reducing \(\beta\) will mitigate the drop-off in selection probabilities from large studies to small ones.

Comments

There are several aspects of the Copas model that I find somewhat perplexing. One is the assumption that the selection propensity index is correlated with the sampling errors rather than with the marginal distribution of \(T_i^*\). Taken literally, this means that the selection propensity depends not just on the effect size estimate (which is an observable quantity) but on where that estimate lies relative to the true study-specific effect size parameter \(\theta_i\). It seems more plausible to me that authors, reviewers, editors, etc. would make decisions that could influence selective reporting based on observable quantities such as effect size estimates, standard errors, or \(p\)-values (as in the step-function selection model).

Another perplexing aspect of the model is that, for a given set of selection parameters \(\alpha\), \(\beta\), and \(\rho\), the bias due to selection is constant and unaffected by the location \(\mu\) or scale \(\tau\) of the true effect size distribution. As a result, under the model’s posited form of selection bias, the extent of bias is disconnected from considerations of statistical significance or power. In practice, I would expect the risk of selection bias to be greater for small studies examining small effects than for small studies examining very strong effect sizes, but the parameterization of the Copas model makes it kind of hard to capture such a pattern. Put another way, I think this implies that the plausibility of a given selection parameters \(\alpha\), \(\beta\), and \(\rho\) might actually depend on \(\mu\) and \(\tau\).

Third, a well known aspect of the model is that its three selection-related parameters are only very weakly identifiable from observed effect size distributions. Because of this, the Copas model is typically used as a sort of sensitivity analysis, where the analyst estimates \(\mu\), \(\tau\), and \(\rho\) by maximum likelihood (Copas & Shi, 2001) for specified values for \(\alpha\) and \(\beta\), and then examines how these parameter estimates vary across a range of possible \(\alpha\) and \(\beta\).3 But sensitivity analysis is only really helpful to the extent that the analyst can specify a plausible range of values for the selection parameters, which is hard to do if the plausible range depends on \(\mu\) and \(\tau\). Other scholars have proposed using the Copas in a Bayesian framework (e.g., Bai et al., 2021; Mavridis et al., 2013), but it seems like similar concerns would apply—if key parameters are only weakly identified, then their priors may be strongly influential, but if it’s hard to formulas credible priors….

To be clear, my comments here are all just half-baked impressions as I work through a model that’s pretty new to me. Copas has some more recent work that considers alternative assumptions about the selective reporting process (Copas, 2013), which seem simpler and more closely connected to \(p\)-value-based selection processes. However, other scholars have continued to study and extend the Copas selection model (e.g., Duan et al., 2020; Ning et al., 2017; Piao et al., 2019), so I probably just need to read more about how it’s applied to get a better sense of its strengths and utility.

Back to top

References

Arellano-Valle, R. B., & Azzalini, A. (2006). On the unification of families of skew-normal distributions. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 33(3), 561–574. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9469.2006.00503.x
Arnold, B. C., Beaver, R. J., Groeneveld, R. A., & Meeker, W. Q. (1993). The nontruncated marginal of a truncated bivariate normal distribution. Psychometrika, 58(3), 471–488. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02294652
Bai, R., Lin, L., Boland, M. R., & Chen, Y. (2021). A robust Bayesian Copas selection model for quantifying and correcting publication bias. arXiv. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2005.02930
Bom, P. R. D., & Rachinger, H. (2019). A kinked meta-regression model for publication bias correction. Research Synthesis Methods, 10(4), 497–514. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1352
Copas, J. B. (1999). What works?: Selectivity models and meta-analysis. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series A: Statistics in Society, 162(1), 95–109. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-985X.00123
Copas, J. B. (2013). A likelihood-based sensitivity analysis for publication bias in meta-analysis. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series C: Applied Statistics, 62(1), 47–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9876.2012.01049.x
Copas, J. B., & Li, H. G. (1997). Inference for non-random samples. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B: Statistical Methodology, 59(1), 55–95. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9868.00055
Copas, J. B., & Shi, J. Q. (2000). Meta-analysis, funnel plots and sensitivity analysis. Biostatistics, 1(3), 247–262. https://doi.org/10.1093/biostatistics/1.3.247
Copas, J. B., & Shi, J. Q. (2001). A sensitivity analysis for publication bias in systematic reviews. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 10, 251–265.
Duan, R., Piao, J., Marks-Anglin, A., Tong, J., Lin, L., Chu, H., Ning, J., & Chen, Y. (2020). Testing for publication bias in meta-analysis under Copas selection model. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2007.00836
Hedges, L. V. (1992). Modeling publication selection effects in meta-analysis. Statistical Science, 7(2), 246–255.
Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (1996). Estimating effect size under publication bias: Small sample properties and robustness of a random effects selection model. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 21(4), 299. https://doi.org/10.3102/10769986021004299
Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (2005). Selection method approaches. In Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment, and adjustments (pp. 145–174). John Wiley & Sons.
Huang, A., Komukai, S., Friede, T., & Hattori, S. (2021). Using clinical trial registries to inform Copas selection model for publication bias in meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 12(5), 658–673. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1506
Mavridis, D., Sutton, A., Cipriani, A., & Salanti, G. (2013). A fully Bayesian application of the Copas selection model for publication bias extended to network meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 32(1), 51–66. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.5494
Ning, J., Chen, Y., & Piao, J. (2017). Maximum likelihood estimation and EM algorithm of Copas-like selection model for publication bias correction. Biostatistics, 18(3), 495–504. https://doi.org/10.1093/biostatistics/kxx004
Piao, J., Liu, Y., Chen, Y., & Ning, J. (2019). Copas-like selection model to correct publication bias in systematic review of diagnostic test studies. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 28(10-11), 2912–2923. https://doi.org/10.1177/0962280218791602
Stanley, T. D. (2008). Meta-regression methods for detecting and estimating empirical effects in the presence of publication selection*. Oxford Bulletin of Economics and Statistics, 70(1), 103–127. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0084.2007.00487.x
Vevea, J. L., & Hedges, L. V. (1995). A general linear model for estimating effect size in the presence of publication bias. Psychometrika, 60(3), 419–435. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02294384
Vevea, J. L., & Woods, C. M. (2005). Publication bias in research synthesis: Sensitivity analysis using a priori weight functions. Psychological Methods, 10(4), 428–443. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.10.4.428

Footnotes

  1. Note that I’ve plotted the unnormalized density (the density before dividing by \(\Phi\left(\alpha + \frac{\beta}{\sigma_i^*}\right)\)) so that it’s clear that the distribution of observed effects is a subset of the distribution of effects prior to selection.↩︎

  2. These sources give the variance of the observed effect size estimates as \(\mathbb{V}\left(T_i | \sigma_i \right) = \sigma_i^2\left(1 - \rho^2 \lambda(u_i) \times [u_i + \lambda(u_i)]\right)\) (see p. 250 of Copas & Shi, 2000; see Equation 6 of Copas & Shi, 2001). However, these expressions are incorrect as stated because they omit the between-study heterogeneity \(\tau^2\).↩︎

  3. Some recent work by Huang et al. (2021) proposes using clinical trial registry information to estimate all the parameters of the Copas model, which seems like an interesting and potentially useful strategy.↩︎